Advances in Applied Psychology
Articles Information
Advances in Applied Psychology, Vol.1, No.1, Aug. 2015, Pub. Date: Jul. 29, 2015
On Arabic Abstract and Concrete Words Recall Using Free, Cued, and Serial Recall Paradigms: Is It Abstractness, Concreteness, or Zero Effect
Pages: 51-72 Views: 5601 Downloads: 1255
Authors
[01] Ahmed Mohammed Saleh Alduais, Department of Linguistics, Social Sciences Institute Ankara University, Sıhhiye, Ankara, Turkey.
[02] Yasir Saad Almukhaizeem, Department of Linguistics and Translation, College of Languages and Translation, King Saud University, Riyadh, Kingdom of Saudi Arabia.
Abstract
Purpose: To see in which type of recall abstract and/or concrete words are better recalled, and if there are any differences between types of recall that would affect recall of abstract words over concrete ones or vice versa. Methods: Three groups of undergraduates in King Saud University, Saudi Arabia participated in this study where they were trained to differentiate between abstract and concrete words. A list of 20 Arabic abstract and concrete words was then given to them to be classified into abstract and concrete words based on four factors: concreteness, imageability, meaningfulness, and age of acquisition. An observation sheet was provided to the experiment administrator to document observed recall effects and recalled words. Three methods were used to facilitate this experiment: auditory, visual, and hand-writing methods. Results: Both referential and descriptive statistical tools were run to analyse the collected data and answer the questions of the current study. Run referential statistical tools approved the usability of the designed measures. Descriptive statistical tools revealed that the participants in the free recall group remembered all words in the 20 list, 10 list, abstract and concrete ones. In the cued recall group, they remembered the over majority of the words in the case of concrete ones but not all the abstract ones. Again, for the serial recall group, they remembered only few words be it in the case of short list, long list, abstract or concrete with more remembered words in the case of abstract words. Zero effect was the most frequent effect among the three groups (free recall= 100, cued recall= 98.5 and serial recall= 82.3). Conclusions: Our results indicated that abstract and concrete words are better recalled in free recall paradigms than in cued and serial recall paradigms. Yet, words are more poorly and considerably recalled in serial recall than in both free and cued recall paradigms. There was also neither an advantage of concrete words over abstract ones nor for abstract words over concrete ones albeit insignificant statistical difference was calculated in the case of cued and serial recall paradigms in favour of abstractness effect. Finally, while recency effect is more frequent than primacy effect during free recall paradigms, both forward and backward recall effects are similar to one another in frequency level during cued recall paradigms. Transposition gradients effect is the most frequent observed effect during serial recall paradigms among the possible types of effects.
Keywords
Abstract Words, Concrete Words, Free Recall, Cued Recall, Serial Recall, Recall Effects, Abstractness Effect, Concreteness Effect, Zero Effect
References
[01] Baker, R., Tehan, G., & Tehan, H. (2012). Word length and age influences on forward and backward immediate serial recall. Memory & Cognition, 40(1), 40-51. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/952889439?accountid=142908
[02] Binder, J. R., Westbury, C. F., McKiernan, K. A., Possing, E. T., & Medler, D. A. (2005). Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(6), pp. 905-17.
[03] Borghi, A. M., Flumini, A., Cimatti, F., Marocco, D., & Scorolli, C. (2011). Manipulating objects and telling words: A study on concrete and abstract words acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 15. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00015
[04] Campos, A. (2009). Imagery Vividness and Emotionality of Concrete and Abstract Words: A Classroom Activity. Revista Galego-Portuguesa De Psicoloxía E Educación, 17(1,2), pp. 1138-1663.
[05] Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K. (2005). Abstract and concrete concepts have structurally different representational frameworks. Brain, 128(pt 3), pp. 615-27.
[06] Dahlstrom, D., & Ultis, J. (2014, March 1). Modelling Reaction Time for Abstract and Concrete Concepts using a Recurrent Network. San Diego, San Diego, USA.
[07] Dukes, W. F., & Bastian, J. (1966). Recall of abstract and concrete words equated for meaningfulness. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 5(5), pp. 455–458.
[08] Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A., Afonso, O., Scheepers, C., & Carreiras, M. (2008). Qualitative differences in the representation of abstract versus concrete words: evidence from the visual-world paradigm. Cognition, 110(2), pp. 284-92.
[09] Farley, A. P., Ramonda, K., & Liu, X. (2012). The concreteness effect and the bilingual lexicon: The impact of visual stimuli attachment on meaning recall of abstract L2 words. Language Teaching Research, 16(4), 449-466. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1362168812436910
[10] Faust, M. (Ed.). (2012). The Handbook of the Neuropsychology of Language. West Sussex: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
[11] Fliessbach, K., Weis, S., Klaver, P., Elger, C. E., & Weber, B. (2006). The effect of word concreteness on recognition memory. NeuroImage, 32(3), 1413-1421. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.007
[12] Friedlander, K. J. (n.d). Abstract, Concrete, General, and Specific Terms. Retrieved from The Guide to Grammar and Writing: http://grammar.ccc.commnet.edu/grammar/composition/abstract.htm
[13] Hanley, J. R., Hunt, R. P., Steed, D. A., & Jackman, S. (2013). Concreteness and word production. Memory & Cognition, 41(3), 365-377. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1347611949?accountid=142908
[14] Harad, L., & Coch, D. (2009). Remembering abstract vs. concrete words. Poster presented at the 18th Annual Wetterhahn Undergraduate Science Poster Symposium, Women in Science Project (WISP), Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire, and USA.
[15] Il'yuchenok ,I.R., Sysoeva, O. V., & Ivanitskii, A. M. (2008). Two semantic systems in the brain for rapid and slow differentiation of abstract and concrete words. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 38(9), 963-70. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11055-008-9083-5
[16] Ise, C., & Altarriba, J. (2009). The word concreteness effect occurs for positive, but not negative, emotion words in immediate serial recall. British Journal of Psychology, 100(1), 91. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/199590890?accountid=142908
[17] Lagishetti, S. K., & Goswami, S. P. (2012). Measurement of Reaction Time for Processing of Concrete and Abstract Words. Journal of All India Institute of Speech and Hearing JAIISH, 31, pp. 139-144.
[18] Landry, J. (2009). Concrete vs. Abstract Words. Retrieved from wier.ca: http://www.wier.ca/attachments/178_ConcreteVs.AbstractWords.pdf
[19] Markowitsch, H. J. (2014). Memory. Retrieved from Springerreference.com: http://www.springerreference.com/docs/html/chapterdbid/350067.html
[20] Marques, J. F., & Nunes, L. D. (2012). The contributions of language and experience to the representation of abstract and concrete words: Different weights but similar organizations. Memory & Cognition, 40(8), 1266-1275. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1266030094?accountid=142908
[21] Mastin, L. (2010). The Human Memory. Retrieved from human-memory.net: http://www.human-memory.net/index.html
[22] Mestres-Missé, A., Münte, T. F., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2009). Functional neuroanatomy of contextual acquisition of concrete and abstract words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), pp. 2154-71.
[23] MRC-Psycholinguistic-Database-Editors. (1987). MRC Psycholinguistic Database: Dict Utility Interface. Retrieved from MRC Psycholinguistic Database: http://websites.psychology.uwa.edu.au/school/MRCDatabase/uwa_mrc.htm
[24] Ono, T. (2014). Learning and Memory. Retrieved from Springerreference.com: http://www.springerreference.com/docs/html/chapterdbid/116293.html
[25] Paivio, A. (1965). Abstractness, imagery, and meaningfulness in paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, pp. 32-38.
[26] Paivio, A. (1968). A factor-analytic study of word attributes and verbal learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, pp. 41-49.
[27] Paivio, A. (1974). Spacing of Repetitions in the Incidental and Intentional Free. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, pp. 497-511.
[28] Paivio, A. (1975). Coding distinctions and repetition effects in memory. London: Dept. of Psychology, University of Western Ontario.
[29] Paivio, A. (1990). Mental Representations: A Dual Coding Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[30] Paivio, A., & Csapo, K. (1973). Picture superiority in free recall: Imagery or dual coding? Cognitive Psychology, 5, pp. 176-206.
[31] Paivio, A., & Desrochers, A. (1981). A dual-coding approach to bilingual memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 20, pp. 532-539.
[32] Paivio, A., & Foth, D. (1970). Imaginal and verbal mediators and noun concreteness in paired-associate learning: The elusive interaction. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, pp. 384-390.
[33] Paivio, A., & Okovita, H. W. (1971). Word imagery modalities and associative learning in blind and sighted subjects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, pp. 506-510.
[34] Paivio, A., & Yuille, J. C. (1966). Word abstractness and meaningfulness, and paired-associate learning in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 4(1), 81-89. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/84317893?accountid=142908
[35] Pobric, G., Lambon Ralph, M., A., & Jefferies, E. (2009). The role of the anterior temporal lobes in the comprehension of concrete and abstract words: RTMS evidence. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 45(9), 1104-1110. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.006
[36] Prior, M. R., Cumming, G., & Hendy, J. (1984). Recognition of abstract and concrete words in a dichotic listening paradigm. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 20(1), 149-157. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/81071377?accountid=142908
[37] Psychology-Dictionary-Editors. (2012). What is abstract word? Retrieved from Psychologydictionary.org: http://psychologydictionary.org/abstract-word/
[38] Psychology-Dictionary-Editors. (2012). What is concrete word? Retrieved from Psychologydictionary.org: http://psychologydictionary.org/concrete-word/
[39] Rastatter, M., Dell, C. W., McGuire, R. A., & Loren, C. (1987). Vocal reaction times to unilaterally presented concrete and abstract words: Towards a theory of differential right hemispheric semantic processing. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 23(1), 135-142. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/77482560?accountid=142908
[40] Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Akin, C. E. (1993). Developmental trends in lexical decisions for abstract and concrete words. Reading research report no. 1. (). Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/62860333?accountid=142908
[41] Schwanenflugel, P. J., Akin, C., & Luh, W. (1992). Context availability and the recall of abstract and concrete words. Memory & Cognition (Pre-2011), 20(1), 96-104. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/217453817?accountid=142908
[42] Schwanenflugel, P. J., Henderson, R. L., & Fabricius, W. V. (1998). Developing organization of mental verbs and theory of mind in middle childhood: Evidence from extensions. Developmental Psychology, 34(3), 512-524. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/62451398?accountid=142908
[43] Walker, I., & Hulme, C. (1999). Concrete words are easier to recall than abstract words: Evidence for a semantic contribution to short-term serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25(5), 1256-1271. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/214370571?accountid=142908
[44] Wang, J., Conder, J. A., Blitzer, D. N., & Shinkareva, S. V. (2010). Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 31(10), pp. 1459-68.
[45] Weiss, S., Müller, H. M., Wörmann, F., & Mertens, M. (2011). "Tooth and Truth": Brain Activation During Passive Listening to Concrete and Abstract Nouns. The Open Behavioral Science Journal, pp. 37-47.
[46] West, W. C., & Holcomb, P. J. (2000). Imaginal, semantic, and surface-level processing of concrete and abstract words: An electrophysiological investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1024-1037. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/72554202?accountid=142908
[47] Wiemer-Hastings, K., & Xu, X. (2005). Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Science, 29(5), 719-736. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/62087284?accountid=142908
[48] Yao, B., Vasiljevic, M., Weick, M., Sereno, M. E., O'Donnell, P.,J., & Sereno, S. C. (2013). Semantic size of abstract concepts: It gets emotional when you can’t see it. PLoS One, 8(9) doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075000
600 ATLANTIC AVE, BOSTON,
MA 02210, USA
+001-6179630233
AIS is an academia-oriented and non-commercial institute aiming at providing users with a way to quickly and easily get the academic and scientific information.
Copyright © 2014 - American Institute of Science except certain content provided by third parties.