American Journal of Psychology and Cognitive Science
Articles Information
American Journal of Psychology and Cognitive Science, Vol.1, No.2, Jun. 2015, Pub. Date: Jun. 24, 2015
Brain Activity and Special Relativity: Estimation and a Novel Hypothesis to Explain Time Perception
Pages: 66-74 Views: 4281 Downloads: 1753
Authors
[01] Amir Hosein Ghaderi, Cognitive Neuroscience Lab., University of Tabriz, Tabriz, Iran.
Abstract
The theory of special relativity suggests that, time is a byproduct of velocity and it is created during movement, obligatory. Subjective time is perceived during all senses and this perception also is obligatory. In this paper, I suppose that psychological perceived time is analogous to physical relative time. The brain uses many neural pathways in sensory system for perception. Overall the length of these pathways is very large and the information network is very huge. On the other hand, binding in this network is occurred in very little time. So the velocity of data transfer and integration in this network is too high. I suggest that time perception is related to this high speed. In this paper the internal clock and other dedicated models have been considered as Newtonian timing systems (an invalid theory). Also two time perception models which are based on the theory of special relativity are criticized and a novel hypothesis based on special relativity and brain activity is presented. The proposed hypothesis suggests that, the velocity of integration in the human cortex is near the speed of light and subjective time dilation and compression is occurred due to this relativistic speed. Many time distortions during psychological tasks and many physiological evidences are consistent with this novel hypothesis.
Keywords
Time Perception, Einstein Special Relativity, Brain Activity, Integration
References
[01] Newton, I. Sir Isaac Newton's mathematical principles of natural philosophy and his system of the world. (University of California Press, Berkeley, 1934).
[02] Einstein, A. (1920). Relativity: The Special and the General Theory, (trans. Lawson, R. W.) (Methuen, London, 1920).
[03] Treisman, M. Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the" internal clock". Psychological Monographs: General and Applied 77, 1-31 (1963).
[04] Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P. L., Brogan, D. The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception 19, 705-743 (1990).
[05] Ivry, R. B., Schlerf, J. E. Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends in cognitive sciences 12, 273-280(2008).
[06] Eagleman, D. M., Pariyadath, V. Is subjective duration a signature of coding efficiency?. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1841-1851 (2009).
[07] Allman, M. J., Meck, W. H. Pathophysiological distortions in time perception and timed performance. Brain 135, 656-677 (2012).
[08] Pouthas, V. et al. Neural network involved in time perception: an fMRI study comparing long and short interval estimation. Human brain mapping 25, 433-441 (2005).
[09] Sadeghi, N. G., Pariyadath, V., Apte, S., Eagleman, D. M., Cook, E. P. Neural correlates of subsecond time distortion in the middle temporal area of visual cortex. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 23, 3829-3840 (2011).
[10] Craig, A. D. How do you feel now? the anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10, 59-70 (2009).
[11] Wittmann, M. The inner sense of time: how the brain creates a representation of duration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14, 217-223 (2013).
[12] Karmarkar, U. R., Buonomano, D. V. Timing in the absence of clocks: encoding time in neural network states. Neuron 53, 427-438 (2007).
[13] Buonomano, D. V., Maass, W. State-dependent computations: spatiotemporal processing in cortical networks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10, 113-125 (2009).
[14] Wittmann, M. The inner experience of time. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364, 1955-1967 (2009).
[15] Henson, R. N. A., Rugg, M. D. Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming. Neuropsychologia 41, 263-270 (2003).
[16] Summerfield, C., Trittschuh, E. H., Monti, J. M., Mesulam, M. M., Egner, T. Neural repetition suppression reects fulfilled perceptual expectations. Nature neuroscience 11, 1004-1006 (2008).
[17] Terao, M., Watanabe, J., Yagi, A., Nishida, S. Y. Reduction of stimulus visibility compresses apparent time intervals. Nature neuroscience 11, 541-542 (2008).
[18] Kinoshita, M., Komatsu, H. Neural representation of the luminance and brightness of a uniform surface in the macaque primary visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 86, 2559-2570 (2001).
[19] Eagleman, D. M. Human time perception and its illusions. Current opinion in neurobiology 18, 131-136 (2008).
[20] Pariyadath, V., Eagleman, D. The effect of predictability on subjective duration. Plos one 2, e1264 (2007).
[21] Morrone, M. C., Ross, J., Burr, D. C. in Space and time in perception and action (eds Nijhawan, R., Khurana, B.) 52-62 (Cambridge University Press, New York 2010).
[22] Burr, D. C., Ross, J., Binda, 276 P., Morrone, M. C. Saccades compress space, time and number. Trends in cognitive sciences 14, 528-533 (2010).
[23] Allman, M. J., Teki, S., Griffiths, T. D., Meck, W. H. Properties of the internal clock: first-and second-order principles of subjective time. Annual review of psychology 65, 743-771 (2014).
[24] Meck, W. H., Penney, T. B., Pouthas, V. Cortico-striatal representation of time in animals and humans. Current opinion in neurobiology 18, 145-152 (2008).
[25] Capria, M. M., Superluminal Waves and Objects: Theory and Experiments. A Panoramic. Physics Before and After Einstein, 267 (2005).
[26] Burr, D. C., Morrone, M. C., Ross, J. Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements. Nature 371, 511-513 (1994).
[27] Ross, J., Burr, D., Morrone, C. Suppression of the magnocellular pathway during saccades. Behavioural brain research 80, 1-8 (1996).
[28] Thiele, A., Henning, P., Kubischik, M., Hoffmann, K. P. Neural mechanisms of saccadic suppression. Science 295, 2460-2462 (2002).
[29] Kleiser, R., Seitz, R. J., Krekelberg, B. Neural correlates of saccadic suppression in humans. Current Biology 14, 386-390 (2004).
[30] Yarrow, K., Haggard, P., Heal, R., Brown, P., Rothwell, J. C. Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity. Nature 414, 302-305 (2001).
[31] Morrone, M. C., Ross, J., Burr, D. Saccadic eye movements cause compression of time as well as space. Nature neuroscience 8, 950-954 (2005).
[32] Eagleman, D. M. Distortions of time during rapid eye movements. Nature neuroscience 8, 850-851 (2005).
[33] Henson, R., Shallice, T., Dolan, R. Neuroimaging evidence for dissociable forms of repetition priming. Science 287, 1269-1272 (2000).
[34] Dobbins, I. G., Schnyer, D. M., Verfaellie, M., Schacter, D. L. Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning. Nature 428, 316-319 (2004).
[35] Wagner, A. D., Desmond, J. E., Demb, J. B., Glover, G. H., Gabrieli, J. D. Semantic repetition priming for verbal and pictorial knowledge: A functional MRI study of left inferior prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9, 714-726 (1997).
[36] McMahon, D. B., Olson, C. R. Repetition suppression in monkey inferotemporal cortex: relation to behavioral priming. Journal of neurophysiology 97, 3532-3543 (2007).
[37] Chong, T. T. J., Cunnington, R., Williams, M. A., Kanwisher, N., Mattingley, J. B. fMRI adaptation reveals mirror neurons in human inferior parietal cortex. Current biology 18, 1576-1580 (2008).
[38] Pariyadath, V., Eagleman, D. M. Subjective duration distortions mirror neural repetition suppression. Plos one 7, e49362 (2012).
[39] Kanai, R., Paffen, C. L., Hogendoorn, H., Verstraten, F. A. Time dilation in dynamic visual display. Journal of Vision 6, 1421-1430 (2006).
[40] Murray, S. O., Boyaci, H., Kersten, D. The representation of perceived angular size in human primary visual cortex. Nature neuroscience 9, 429-434 (2006).
[41] Buhusi, C. V., Meck, W. H. Relativity theory and time perception: single or multiple clocks?. Plos one 4, e6268 (2009).
[42] Kropotov, J. Quantitative EEG, event-related potentials and neurotherapy. (Academic Press, 2010).
[43] Pakkenberg, B., Gundersen, H. J. G. Neocortical neuron number in humans: e_ect of sex and age. Journal of Comparative Neurology 384, 312-320 (1997).
[44] Laughlin, S. B., Sejnowski, T. J. Communication in neuronal networks. Science 301, 1870-1874 (2003).
[45] Pakken319 berg, B. et al. Aging and the human neocortex. Experimental gerontology 38, 95-99 (2003).
[46] Marner, L., Nyengaard, J. R., Tang, Y., Pakkenberg, B. Marked loss of myelinated nerve fibers in the human brain with age. Journal of Comparative Neurology 462, 144-152 (2003).
[47] Jensen, O., Kaiser, J., Lachaux, J. P. Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory. Trends in neurosciences 30, 317-324 (2007).
[48] Kandel, E., Barres, B., Hudspeth A. J. in Principles of Neural Science, (eds Kandel, E., Schwartz, J., Jessell, T., Siegelbaum, S., Hudspeth A. J.) 21-38 (McGraw Hill Professional, New York, 2013)
[49] Wittmann, M. Moments in time. Frontiers in integrative neuroscience 5, 66 (2011).
[50] Repp, B. H. On the nature of phase attraction in sensorimotor synchronization with interleaved auditory sequences. Human Movement Science 23(3), 389-413 (2004).
[51] Giraud, A. L., Kleinschmidt, A., Poeppel, D., Lund, T. E., Frackowiak, R. S., Laufs, H. Endogenous cortical rhythms determine cerebral specialization for speech perception and production. Neuron 56(6), 1127-1134 (2007)
600 ATLANTIC AVE, BOSTON,
MA 02210, USA
+001-6179630233
AIS is an academia-oriented and non-commercial institute aiming at providing users with a way to quickly and easily get the academic and scientific information.
Copyright © 2014 - American Institute of Science except certain content provided by third parties.